TY - JOUR
T1 - Taking a language stance
AU - Cowley, Stephen J.
N1 - Original article can be found at : http://www.tandfonline.com/ Copyright Taylor & Francis [Full text of this article is not available in the UHRA]
PY - 2011
Y1 - 2011
N2 - Linguists tend to view language in terms of forms and their use. For historical reasons, speaking and listening are often ascribed to knowledge of a language system. Language behavior is seen as the production and processing of forms. Others contrast language to man-made codes (see Kravchenko, 2007; Love, 2004). Instead of focusing on forms, language can be conceived of as action and, as such, both dynamic and symbolic (Raczaszek-Leonardi, 2009). History places us in a meshwork where public resources of language, among other things, contribute to games, mashing beans, and watching television. Speaking-while-hearing draws on cultural products (e.g., axes, social roles, pictures, and wordings). As we collaborate, we orient to wordings or repeated (and systematized) aspects of vocalizations that, within our community, carry historically derived information. Pursuing this view, it is argued that hearing "words" is like seeing "things" in pictures. This is described as taking a language stance. To defend the position, it is argued that, first, we learn to hear wordings and, later, to use "what we hear" as ways of constraining our actions. Far from depending on individual knowledge, orienting to wordings makes language irreducibly collective.
AB - Linguists tend to view language in terms of forms and their use. For historical reasons, speaking and listening are often ascribed to knowledge of a language system. Language behavior is seen as the production and processing of forms. Others contrast language to man-made codes (see Kravchenko, 2007; Love, 2004). Instead of focusing on forms, language can be conceived of as action and, as such, both dynamic and symbolic (Raczaszek-Leonardi, 2009). History places us in a meshwork where public resources of language, among other things, contribute to games, mashing beans, and watching television. Speaking-while-hearing draws on cultural products (e.g., axes, social roles, pictures, and wordings). As we collaborate, we orient to wordings or repeated (and systematized) aspects of vocalizations that, within our community, carry historically derived information. Pursuing this view, it is argued that hearing "words" is like seeing "things" in pictures. This is described as taking a language stance. To defend the position, it is argued that, first, we learn to hear wordings and, later, to use "what we hear" as ways of constraining our actions. Far from depending on individual knowledge, orienting to wordings makes language irreducibly collective.
U2 - 10.1080/10407413.2011.591272
DO - 10.1080/10407413.2011.591272
M3 - Article
SN - 1040-7413
VL - 23
SP - 185
EP - 209
JO - Ecological Psychology
JF - Ecological Psychology
IS - 3
ER -